In 2007, internationally acclaimed musician Joshua Bell, illustrated the role of context in the enjoyment of music when he played his Stradivarius violin in a Washington, D.C., subway, and commuters passed by without a second glance.
A new study probing the role that expectations and biases play in our experiences has found that simply being told a performer is a professional or a student changes the way the brain responds to music, and that overcoming this bias takes a deliberate effort.
The results of the study, by researchers at the University of Connecticut, the University of Arkansas, and Arizona State University, are published today in Scientific Reports.
The study involved 20 participants without formal training in music. Inside a functional magnetic imaging, or fMRI, machine at the Brain Imaging Research Center at the University of Connecticut, the participants listened to eight pairs of 70-second musical excerpts, presented in a random order. Each pair consisted of two different performances of the same excerpt. The participants were told that one of the pairs was played by a “conservatory student of piano” and the other was a “world-renowned professional pianist.” Although participants were actually listening to a student and professional performance, they heard each pair twice during the experiment with the labels reversed, ensuring that the researchers could investigate the effect of the label independent of the qualities of the performance itself.
Participants rated their enjoyment of each excerpt on a scale of one to 10, and they indicated which of the two excerpts in each pair they preferred. The researchers used the fMRI scans to examine regions of the brain that are associated with auditory processing, pleasure and reward, and cognitive control.
In order to study the brain activity associated with bias, the researchers compared brain images of the participants who preferred the “professional” excerpts with images of participants who preferred the “student” excerpts. They found that when a participant preferred the piece attributed to a professional player, there was significantly more activity in the primary auditory cortex, as well as a region of the brain associated with pleasure and reward.
This activity started when the participant was informed that the player was a professional – before the music even began – and remained consistent during the excerpt, suggesting that the belief that a musician is a professional caused these participants to pay more attention to the music, and biased their listening experience not just at the start, but throughout the excerpt.
The researchers also examined the brain activity of participants who preferred the “student” recordings over the “professional” recordings. While these participants were listening to the recordings attributed to the professional, researchers saw higher activity in a region of the brain related to cognitive control and deliberative thinking throughout the course of the excerpt. They also found that these participants had more connectivity between the parts of their brain related to cognitive control and reward.
“It was different when people listened carefully enough to realize we were fooling them – that is, when they realized they liked the performance labeled ‘student’ better,” says Edward Large, a theoretical neuroscientist at UConn who made the fMRI available for the study.
“The participants who could resist the bias (who decided they liked the performance primed as student, or disliked the one primed as professional) had to recruit regions devoted to executive control – it looked like work for them to suppress the bias,” says Elizabeth Margulis, distinguished professor of music theory and music cognition at the University of Arkansas. “These data demonstrate how critical factors outside the notes themselves, like the information you have about a performer (explicitly in the form of a prime, or implicitly in the form of positioning on stage at Carnegie Hall or on a subway platform) can transform what you are able to hear and how you evaluate a musical performance.”
These findings have implications beyond music preference, the researchers explained.
“Our findings are relevant for behavioral economists, psychologists, and artists alike,” they wrote in the paper, “as they demonstrate that ‘deliberative and effortful thinking’ can play a crucial role in overcoming cognitive heuristics related to socially constructed concepts and stereotypes.”